Elsevier

Neuroscience

Volume 336, 12 November 2016, Pages 1-11
Neuroscience

The medial frontal cortex contributes to but does not organize rat exploratory behavior

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.08.041Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Rats use self-movement cues to organize their exploratory behavior.

  • Medial frontal lesions disrupt exploratory behavior under light and dark conditions.

  • Behavioral disruptions are consistent with impaired motor coordination.

  • Behavioral disruptions are consistent with impaired egocentric processing.

  • Medial frontal cortex is required, but not sufficient for idiothetic cue processing.

Abstract

Animals use multiple strategies to maintain spatial orientation. Dead reckoning is a form of spatial navigation that depends on self-movement cue processing. During dead reckoning, the generation of self-movement cues from a starting position to an animal’s current position allow for the estimation of direction and distance to the position movement originated. A network of brain structures has been implicated in dead reckoning. Recent work has provided evidence that the medial frontal cortex may contribute to dead reckoning in this network of brain structures. The current study investigated the organization of rat exploratory behavior subsequent to medial frontal cortex aspiration lesions under light and dark conditions. Disruptions in exploratory behavior associated with medial frontal lesions were consistent with impaired motor coordination, response inhibition, or egocentric reference frame. These processes are necessary for spatial orientation; however, they are not sufficient for self-movement cue processing. Therefore it is possible that the medial frontal cortex provides processing resources that support dead reckoning in other brain structures but does not of itself compute the kinematic details of dead reckoning.

Introduction

Many neurological disorders are associated with disruptions in spatial orientation (Aguirre and D’Esposito, 1999, Tetewsky and Duffy, 1999, O’Brien et al., 2001). For example, patients that have experienced stroke often exhibit impairments in spatial orientation (Meerwaldt and Van Harskamp, 1982). Specifically, patients with damage to frontal cortical areas have been reported to display lateralized impairments in their ability to process egocentric spatial information, known as spatial neglect (Mesulam, 1990, Heilman et al., 1993). One spatial orientation strategy, dead reckoning, involves the online processing of self-movement cues (i.e., vestibular, proprioception, efferent copies of action commands) to estimate direction and distance to the point that movement originated (Gallistel, 1990). Work on humans (Worsley et al., 2001, Philbeck et al., 2004, Shrager et al., 2008, Kim et al., 2013) and rats (Maaswinkel et al., 1999, Wallace and Whishaw, 2003, Winter et al., 2013, Kim et al., 2013; however, see Alyan and McNaughton, 1999) has provided support for these cues being processed by a network of brain structures that include the hippocampal formation and hippocampal cholinergic (Martin and Wallace, 2007) and GABAergic (Köppen et al., 2013) projections that originate in the medial septum. In addition, several other cortical areas contribute to dead reckoning including the entorhinal (Parron and Save, 2004, Winter et al., 2013, Van Cauter et al., 2012), parietal (Parron and Save, 2004), and retrosplenial (Cooper et al., 2001, Whishaw et al., 2001a) cortical areas.

Imaging work has provided support for expanding this network of structures to include the medial frontal cortex (Wolbers et al., 2007). Human participants were given a dead reckoning navigation task during an fMRI scan that required participants to indicate the direction of the start location after traveling a route that involved a change in heading. Hippocampal and medial frontal cortex activity was associated with performance on this virtual dead reckoning task. In contrast, previous rat work has demonstrated that medial frontal cortex lesions spare performance on a task that depends on self-movement cue processing (Whishaw et al., 2001a). In the rat study, the animals were given many trials in which they carried food to a home base and it is possible that the reinforcement contingencies of the task may have allowed the animals to compensate for impairments in self-movement recognition. The goal of the current study, therefore, is to examine the contribution of the medial frontal cortex in a dead reckoning task that involves the computation of spatial position from spontaneous exploratory movement similar to that given to human participants in the virtual dead reckoning task.

Rat exploratory behavior is organized at several levels. At a macro level, rats introduced to a novel environment will first establish a home base or a location associated with stopping behavior, long dwell times, and grooming behavior (Whishaw et al., 1983, Eilam and Golani, 1989, Golani et al., 1993, Clark et al., 2005). At a micro level, rats organize their movements around the home base location (Eilam and Golani, 1989, Drai et al., 2000). Movements away from the home base are typically slow and more circuitous relative to movement sequences directed back to the home base (Tchernichovski et al., 1998). Returns to the home base are characterized by a consistent temporal pacing of moment-to-moment speeds that are scaled to the distance between the rats’ current location and their home base (Wallace et al., 2006, Wallace et al., 2006). This organization of the exploratory trip into outward and homeward segments has been observed independent of environmental cue availability—consistent with self-movement cue processing guiding exploratory behavior. Specifically, self-movement cues generated after departing the home base are processed within the temporal context they occur to update an online representation of the current direction and distance to the home base (Barlow, 1964, Mittelstaedt and Mittelstaedt, 1980, Gallistel, 1990, Biegler, 2000, Etienne and Jeffery, 2004, Wallace et al., 2008). Research has shown that damage to the hippocampus (Wallace et al., 2002, Wallace and Whishaw, 2003) or related structures (Martin et al., 2007, Winter et al., 2013) disrupts the direction and distance component of the homeward segment. Therefore, the current study examines the effects of damage, localized to structures within the medial frontal cortex, on the organization of exploratory behavior.

Section snippets

Animals

Twelve adult naïve female Long–Evans rats (University of Lethbridge vivarium), weighing 250–300 g, were housed in groups of two in Plexiglas cages. The colony room was maintained at 20–21 °C with a 12-h/12-h light/dark cycle. Rats were provided ad lib access to food and water in their home cages throughout the duration of the experiment.

Apparatus

The apparatus was a circular table (2.50 m in diameter) without walls mounted on ball bearings that permitted it to be rotated. The surface of the table was 0.64 m

Histology

In all lesion cases except M14, the lesion extended rostro-caudally from the anterior pole of the cortex to the level of the anterior commissure (ac; Fig. 1). In these cases, a bilateral lesion involving the medial frontal cortex was visible beginning at or near the level of the forceps minor (fmi) of the corpus callosum (cc). Lesions at this level involved medial agranular cortex (AgM/M2), cingulate cortex (Cg1), prelimbic cortex (PrL) bilaterally. In all of these cases, the white matter of

Discussion

This study examined the effects of medial frontal lesions on exploratory behavior under light and dark conditions. Both groups established a home base in the refuge and organized their movements around that location. In general, group differences were restricted to the homeward segment and did not depend on the testing condition. For example, the medial frontal group exhibited homeward segments that were highly circuitous and followed the periphery of the table. Several exceptions were noted.

Conclusion

A growing literature has identified the neural systems that contribute to self-movement cue processing. The hippocampus is one structure that has been implicated in the processing of self-movement cues (Maaswinkel et al., 1999, Wallace and Whishaw, 2003). Once the hippocampus has integrated these self-movement cues, the resulting information may be transferred to motor planning regions in the cortex to organize behavior. Transfer of information could be supported by direct projections from the

Acknowledgment

We thank the anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments.

References (66)

  • J.R. Köppen et al.

    Comparison of direction and distance estimation across spatial tasks: absence of sexually dimorphic self-movement cues processing

    Learn Motiv

    (2015)
  • D.L. Korol et al.

    Shifts in preferred learning strategy across the estrous cycle in female rats

    Horm Behav

    (2004)
  • M.M. Martin et al.

    Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation impairs self-movement cue use during a food hoarding task

    Behav Brain Res

    (2007)
  • M.M. Martin et al.

    Medial septum lesions disrupt exploratory trip organization: evidence for septohippocampal involvement in dead reckoning

    Physiol Behav

    (2007)
  • B.B. McAllister et al.

    The effects of chronic fluoxetine treatment following injury of medial frontal cortex in mice

    Behav Brain Res

    (2015)
  • B. Poucet

    A further characterization of the spatial problem-solving deficit induced by lesions of the medial frontal cortex in the rat

    Behav Brain Res

    (1990)
  • R.F. Salazar et al.

    NMDA lesions in the medial frontal cortex impair the ability to inhibit responses during reversal of a simple spatial discrimination

    Behav Brain Res

    (2004)
  • L.W. Swanson

    A direct projection from Ammon’s horn to prefrontal cortex in the rat

    Brain Res

    (1981)
  • D.G. Wallace et al.

    Quantification of a single exploratory trip reveals hippocampal formation mediated dead reckoning

    J Neurosci Methods

    (2002)
  • I.Q. Whishaw et al.

    Dead reckoning (path integration) requires the hippocampal formation: evidence from spontaneous exploration and spatial learning tasks in light (allothetic) and dark (idiothetic) tests

    Behav Brain Res

    (2001)
  • I.Q. Whishaw et al.

    Deficits in allothetic and idiothetic spatial behavior in rats with posterior cingulate cortex lesions

    Behav Brain Res

    (2001)
  • S. Womack et al.

    Medial frontal cortex lesions: deficits and treatment with nimodipine

    Exp Neurol

    (1993)
  • C.L. Worsley et al.

    Path integration following temporal lobectomy in humans

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • V.M. Afonso et al.

    Medial frontal cortex lesions in the female rat affect sexual and maternal behavior and their sequential organization

    Behav Neurosci

    (2007)
  • G.K. Aguirre et al.

    Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy

    Brain

    (1999)
  • S. Alyan et al.

    Hippocampectomized rats are capable of homing by path integration

    Behav Neurosci

    (1999)
  • S.E. Baran et al.

    Prefrontal cortex lesions and sex differences in fear extinction and perseveration

    Learn Mem

    (2010)
  • J.S. Barlow

    Inertial navigation as a basis for animal navigation

    J Theor Biol

    (1964)
  • R. Biegler

    Possible uses of path integration in animal navigation

    Anim Learn Behav

    (2000)
  • R.W. Brown et al.

    Similarities in the development of place and cue navigation by rats in a swimming pool

    Dev Psychobiol

    (2000)
  • B.G. Cooper et al.

    Finding your way in the dark: the retrosplenial cortex contributes to spatial memory and navigation without visual cues

    Behav Neurosci

    (2001)
  • A.S. Etienne et al.

    Path integration in animals

    Hippocampus

    (2004)
  • F. Ferino et al.

    Anatomical and electrophysiological evidence for a direct projection from Ammon’s horn to the medial frontal cortex in the rat

    Exp Brain Res

    (1987)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text