Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 47, Issue 6, May 2009, Pages 1409-1420
Neuropsychologia

Parietal encoding of action in depth

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.028Get rights and content

Abstract

The posterior parietal cortex is a crucial node in the process of coordinates transformation for the visual control of eye and hand movements. This conviction stems from both neurophysiological studies in the behaving monkey and from the analysis of the consequences of parietal lobe lesions in humans. Despite an extensive literature concerning varying aspects of the composition and control of eye and hand movements, there is little information about the physiological processes responsible for encoding target distance and hand movement in depth or about their control and impairment in parietal patients. This review is an attempt to provide a comprehensive picture from the fragmentary material existing on this issue in the literature. This should serve as a basis for discussion of what we consider to be a prototypical function of the dorsal visuomotor stream in the primate brain, that of encoding eye and hand movement in depth.

Introduction

The idea that the visual system is divided into two main streams of information is an old one, and can be tracked back to the work of Max Schultze (1866) and to the Duplicity Theory proposed in 1894 by von Kries (1894).

More recently, Ungerleider and Miskin (1982) suggested that visual processing in the cerebral cortex uses a dorsal visual stream, devoted to the process of spatial localization of objects, and a ventral stream more concerned with the identification of object features, such as form, color, etc., in line with earlier work by Schneider (1969) showing that a similar dichotomy could be found when comparing the consequences of lesions affecting cortical and subcortical visual centers in the hamster. Ungerleider and Miskin's hypothesis was based on behavioral observations, but also benefited from a large amount of data available on cortico-cortical connectivity. In the early nineties, an interesting extension and specification of these ideas on the functional organization of the visual system was proposed by Goodale and Milner (1992), who suggested that the neural substrates of visual perception may be distinct from those underlying visual mechanisms used to guide motor behaviour. According to their view, the dorsal stream projecting from the primary visual cortex (V1) to the parietal lobe through extrastriate areas would be more involved in visual processing for the control of action, while the ventral stream projecting to the inferotemporal cortex would be more concerned with visual perception. These ideas were quasi-contemporary to the description of the anatomical and functional organization of the parieto-frontal network underlying arm reaching (Caminiti, Johnson & Ferraina, 1996; Johnson, Ferraina, & Caminiti 1993; Johnson, Ferraina, Bianchi, & Caminiti, 1996), and with the demonstration offered by these studies that the main source of visual information to the monkeys frontal areas controlling limb movement was the Superior Parietal Lobule (SPL), a cortical region directly projecting to premotor and motor cortices. Therefore, these anatomical and physiological studies in monkeys were along the same line of reasoning proposed by Goodale and Milner on the role of the dorsal stream.

The neuropsychological evidence supporting the existence of a dorsal visuomotor channel, as opposed to a ventral one, derives first from the observation of a patient (DF) suffering from carbon monoxide poisoning affecting the lateral and ventral prestriate areas (Milner et al., 1991) and, probably disconnecting V1 from the inferotemporal cortex. Patient DF showed normal ability to use visual attributes to perform movements, while the same attributes could not be reported by her in naming, matching or forced choice tasks (Goodale, Milner, Jakobson, & Carey, 1991; Milner et al., 1991).

Directly related to the subject of our review is the observation that in patient DF normal reaching performance was based on intact sensitivity to distance information and to binocular cues, such as the eyes vergence angle (Carey, Dijkerman, & Milner, 1998; Carey, Jacobson, & Goodale, 1991; Mon-Williams, Tresilian, McIntosh, & Milner, 2001) and retinal disparity (Mon-Williams et al., 2001). Similar observations have been obtained in a different patient with visual agnosia (patient DM; Turnbull, Driver, & McCarthy, 2004), displaying an important deficit in depth processing for pictorial perception, but normal depth perception for action. Thus, the mechanisms of encoding depth features for the composition of motor commands seem to survive lesions of the ventral stream.

Conversely, optic ataxia's patients, characterized by lesions centered around the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and SPL (for reviews see Battaglia-Mayer & Caminiti, 2002; Caminiti, Ferraina, Battaglia-Mayer, Mascaro, & Burnod, 2005), exhibit a specific deficit in localizing visual targets with respect to their body (Rondot, de Recondo, & Dumas, 1977). These patients are able to identify objects properly although they cannot accurately perform a goal directed action. More in general, patients with Posterior Parietal Cortex (PPC) lesions are able to recognize objects, but not their spatial relationship (Andersen, 1987, Critchley, 1953), especially in depth (Brain, 1941). In patient WF (Holmes & Horrax, 1919) the parietal cortex lesion caused a severe disturb of object's perception in depth. Patient WF was unable to state which of two objects was near his body or to an external landmark, as well as incapable to avoid obstacle during reaching (Holmes & Horrax, 1919). Thus in humans normal three-dimensional (3D) visual perception seems to depend on parietal cortex. Furthermore, patients with PPC lesions fail to converge the eyes on near objects (reported by Lynch, Mountcastle, Talbot, & Yin, 1977). In conclusion, neuropsychological data obtained from optic ataxia and visual agnosia patients clearly make the case for a double dissociation between perceptual recognition of objects and object-oriented actions. Furthermore, the dissociation seems particularly evident when considering the 3D signals used for visual perception. Binocular signals, such as angular vergence and retinal disparity, seems to rely on the normal functioning of the dorsal stream, while pictorial cues like texture, illumination gradients, and perspective seems mainly processed along the ventral visual stream.

In monkeys, the inactivation of parietal area CIP (Tsutsui, Taira, & Sakata, 2005), in the caudal part of the IPS produces a selective impairment in the recognition of objects surface orientation. Unfortunately, the available lesion studies in monkeys did not analyze in a systematic and quantitative fashion the effect of cortical lesions on encoding of target distance. Together with the difficulty in distinguishing between encoding of visual target distance and reach amplitude, these studies do not provide a solid basis for a critical evaluation of the consequences of cortical lesion on action in depth in this species. In spite of its importance, and probably due to the inherent difficulty of its study, the neural bases of action in depth remain a neglected topic of modern neuroscience. This stands in contrast to the extensive psychophysical and neurophysiological literature (see also Carey et al., 1998) available on depth perception.

In conclusion, little is known about distance processing for action in patients with brain damage in general, and after parietal lesions in particular, and the same holds true for neurophysiological studies concerning the relevance of depth information for the composition of eye and hand movement.

In this review we will attempt a critical discussion of the available literature in this field. First, we will discuss the importance of binocular signals for deriving an egocentric representation of target distance in parietal cortex, as well as the operations underlying the spatial remapping necessary to provide the constancy of the visual word in front of the continuous change of the fixation point in depth. Then, we will illustrate data showing how in parietal cortex binocular information about target distance can be combined with proprioceptive information about hand position to guide reaching in depth, and the relevance of this combination for the coordinates transformation underlying visually guided reaching. Finally, we will highlight the command and control function for eye and hand movement of the parieto-frontal system, as portion of the dorsal visuomotor stream, and its relevance for understanding the consequence of parietal lesion in humans within the conceptual scheme offered by the two visual systems hypothesis.

Section snippets

Disparity signals in PPC are used for the encoding of disjunctive saccades

Accurate guidance of reaching requires signals related to eye positions in the orbit (gaze angle) and eye movements. In naturalistic settings, the eyes land on the target of an upcoming hand action well before the onset of hand movement (Hayhoe, Shrivastava, Mruczek, & Pelz, 2003; Land, Mennie, & Rusted, 1999). Further, hand actions are more accurate when subjects are looking at the target than anywhere else (Neggers & Bekkering, 1999; Van Donkelaar & Staub, 2000). Successful reaching and

Egocentric coding of target distance: Integration of disparity and vergence in PPC

It is generally assumed that to generate a reaching movement to a visual target the brain has to compare the current hand and target positions within the same frame of reference. The importance of this comparison is supported by the observation, that will be discussed later on in this manuscript, that the initial hand position influences in a profound way the neural activity during all epochs of a reaching movement. The egocentric representation of visual targets in 3D space requires the

Spatial remapping for visual constancy after eye movements in depth

During visual exploration and coordinated eye–hand movement in the 3D space the fixation point changes continuously, both in direction and depth. Since during saccades vision is suppressed or reduced (Bridgeman, Handry, & Starck, 1975; Morrone, Ross, & Burr, 1997; Schlag & Schlag-Rey, 1995; Stark, Kong, Schwartz, & Bridgeman, 1976), after each eye movement the 3D representation of object location (based on retinal disparity and eye position) must be updated to maintain stable the gaze-centered

Hand position signals and the specification of the reach movement vector

Accurate encoding of reaching movements depend on the correct perception of the initial hand position, which in turn depends on information provided by the visual and somatosensory systems (Brown et al., 2003a, Brown et al., 2003b; Caminiti, Johnson, Galli, Ferraina, & Burnod, 1991; Caminiti, Johnson, & Urbano, 1990; Graziano, 1999; Sober & Sabes, 2003; Van Beers et al., 1996, Van Beers et al., 1998). The neural bases and rules of this combination are largely unknown. Psychophysical studies

Hand position signals dominate in the rostral region of PPC

Previous results have shown that SPL neurons are modulated by both limb and eye-position signals. Snyder, Batista, and Andersen (1997) reported that limb position modulates the spatial tuning of neurons in the so-called Parietal Reach Region, even though this modulation was less prominent than that exerted by eye position information. Buneo, Jarvis, Batista, and Andersen (2002) showed that the limb position influence becomes stronger when moving rostrally in the SPL, thus confirming the results

PPC as intermediate stage of the coordinates transformation for reaching

The data presented in the previous sections indicate that multiple signals can be detected in SPL regions (area 5, PE) that, by virtue of their cortico-cortical connections (Johnson et al., 1996; Matelli, Govoni, Galletti, Kutz, & Luppino, 1998), can be considered as advanced stages in the process of coordinates transformation for visual reaching. At the final stage of this transformation, a motor command will specify the direction and distance of reaches. In between, the nature of the parietal

Parietal encoding of hand position and movement in 3D space

The results showed that the activity of most parietal neurons was related in a quasi-monotonic fashion to the hand position in space, within a shoulder-centred spherical coordinates system that specifies the azimuth, elevation, and distance of the hand (Fig. 4). Interestingly, inspection of the population revealed that the activity of individual neurons was mostly tuned to changes in position along one of the three dimensions of the coordinates system. Thus, some neurons mostly encoded the

The on-line control of hand movement trajectory by PPC

The flexibility of motor behaviour allows primates to modify their hand trajectory at any time during movement in 3D space. In the experimental laboratory set this ability is traditionally studied by asking subjects (humans or monkeys) to change in-flight the hand movement trajectory whenever a sudden change of target location occurs. A large number of studies have shown that, given enough time for correction, the hand does not complete its movement toward the first target, but describes a

The parieto-frontal network as a command and control system for hand movement

The cortical region were these studies were performed (area PE) is part of the SPL (area 5) and, based on the functional properties of neurons and to ipsilateral cortico-cortical connectivity (Johnson et al., 1996, Matelli et al., 1998), can be considered as an advanced parietal stage in the coordinates transformation for the composition and control of hand movement trajectory in space (Fig. 3). Area PE is cortico-cortically connected with other visuomotor parietal areas, such as PEc (Marconi

Conclusions

The set of data and concepts described in this chapter highlights the crucial role of the parietal lobe in the composition and control of eye and hand movement in depth. The anatomical organization of cortico-cortical connections of the primate's brain places parietal cortex, in particular the SPL, at the interface between vision and movement. Recent studies of cortico-cortical connectivity in man (Koch et al., 2008; Rushworth, Behrens, & Johansen-Berg, 2006) suggest that as far as the

References (148)

  • H. Johnson et al.

    Motor awareness without perceptual awareness

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • M.S. Landy et al.

    Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: In defense of weak fusion

    Vision Research

    (1995)
  • P. Magne et al.

    Retinal and extra-retinal contribution to position coding

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2002)
  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons

    Science

    (1985)
  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7A of macaque

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1990)
  • R.A. Andersen

    The role of the inferior parietal lobule in spatial perception and visual-motor integration

  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (1997)
  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1983)
  • L.B. Bagesteiro et al.

    Interlimb transfer of load compensation during rapid elbow joint movements

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2005)
  • C. Bard et al.

    Deafferentation and pointing with visual double-step perturbations

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1999)
  • P. Bartelson

    Central intermittency twenty years later

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1966)
  • A. Battaglia-Mayer et al.

    Eye-hand coordination during reaching. II. An analysis of the relationships between visuomanual signals in parietal cortex and parieto-frontal association projections

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2001)
  • A. Battaglia-Mayer et al.

    Early coding of reaching in the parieto-occipital cortex

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (2000)
  • A. Battaglia-Mayer et al.

    Optic ataxia as result of the breakdown of the global tuning fields of parietal neurons

    Brain

    (2002)
  • A. Battaglia-Mayer et al.

    Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2003)
  • G. Blohm et al.

    Computations for geometrically accurate visually guided reaching in 3D space

    Journal of Vision

    (2007)
  • J. Blouin et al.

    Control of rapid arm movements when target position is altered during saccadic suppression

    Journal of Motor Behavior

    (1995)
  • W.R. Brain

    Visual disorientation with special reference to lesions of the right hemisphere

    Brain

    (1941)
  • F. Bremmer et al.

    Eye position effects in monkey cortex. II. Pursuit- and fixation-related activity in posterior parietal areas LIP and 7A

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1997)
  • F. Bremmer et al.

    Eye position effects in monkey cortex. I. Visual and pursuit-related activity in extrastriate areas MT and MST

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1997)
  • P.R. Brotchie et al.

    Head position signals used by parietal neurons to encode locations of visual stimuli

    Nature

    (1995)
  • L.E. Brown et al.

    Limb position drift: Implications for control of posture and movement

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (2003)
  • L.E. Brown et al.

    Movement speed effects on limb position drift

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2003)
  • C.A. Buneo et al.

    Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching

    Nature

    (2002)
  • P. Burbaud et al.

    A quantitative study of neuronal discharge in areas 5, 2, and 4 of the monkey during fast arm movements

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1991)
  • R. Caminiti et al.

    The source of visual information to the primate frontal lobe: A novel role for the superior parietal lobule

    Cerebral Cortex

    (1996)
  • R. Caminiti et al.

    Making arm movements within different parts of space: The premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinates system for reaching to visual targets

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1991)
  • R. Caminiti et al.

    Making arm movements within different parts of space: Dynamic mechanisms in the primate motor cortex

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1990)
  • R. Caminiti et al.

    Parallel fronto-parietal circuits for sensorimotor transformation

  • D.P. Carey et al.

    Distance estimation and size constancy in a patient with visual agnosia

    Canadian Journal of Psychology

    (1991)
  • L.G. Carlton

    Processing visual feedback information for movement control

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance

    (1981)
  • M. Carrozzo et al.

    A hybrid frame of reference for visuomanual coordination

    NeuroReport

    (1994)
  • Y. Coello et al.

    Determination of target distance in a structured environment: Selection of visual information for action

    European Journal of Cognitive Psychology

    (2000)
  • Y.E. Cohen et al.

    A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex

    Nature Review of Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • C.L. Colby et al.

    Space and attention in parietal cortex

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • M. Critchley

    Tactile thought, with special reference to the blind

    Brain

    (1953)
  • V. Della-Maggiore et al.

    Stimulation of the posterior parietal cortex interferes with arm trajectory adjustments during the learning of new dynamics

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • S. Deneve et al.

    Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2001)
  • M. Desmurget et al.

    Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target

    Nature Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • F.H. Duffy et al.

    Somatosensory system: Organizational hierarchy from single units in monkey area 5

    Science

    (1971)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Parieto-frontal gradients and domains underlying eye and hand operations in the action space

      2016, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      In conclusion, the crucial feature of this network is that, although in different proportions and depending on the antero-posterior level considered, cells related to eye and/or hand position are ubiquitous and can be considered as a reservoir or matrix of position representations necessary for the selection of coordinated eye-hand movement (Lacquaniti et al., 1995; Johnson et al., 1996; Snyder et al., 2000; Battaglia-Mayer et al., 2001) underlying actions of different complexity. In fact, hand position signaling is essential to compute the movement vector necessary to compose reach plans toward any target in space and exerts a profound influence on encoding hand movement direction in the motor (Caminiti et al., 1990), premotor (Caminiti et al., 1991; Burnod et al., 1992), and posterior parietal cortex (Lacquaniti et al., 1995; Battaglia-Mayer et al., 2000, 2005; Ferraina et al., 2009a,b), therefore on cell activity of all areas of the parieto-frontal network. Furthermore, the eye position modulation of visual parietal activity in the form of gain field (Zipser and Andersen, 1988; for a review see Andersen and Buneo, 2002) has been proposed as a mechanism underlying a body-centered representation of target location.

    • Through the forest of motor representations

      2016, Consciousness and Cognition
    • Developmental improvements in reaching correction efficiency are associated with an increased ability to represent action mentally

      2015, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Interestingly, this developmental progression coincides with the protracted maturation of the parietal cortices and their projections to frontal structures (see Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston, 2005, for a review) and central nervous system more broadly (Kail, 1991). These neural systems are known to be critical to visually guided reaching in adults (Ferraina, Battaglia-Mayer, Genoveslo, Archambault, & Caminiti, 2009; Pisella, Binkofski, Lasek, Toni, & Rossetti, 2006), with the posterior parietal cortex in particular thought to be critical to integrating sensory (especially visual) inputs with predictive estimates of limb locations (Macuga & Frey, 2014) and possibly involved in generating and processing the error signals that arise following mismatches between the two (Reichenbach, Bresciani, Peer, Bulthoff, & Thielscher, 2011). Empirical support for the argument that improvements in the proficiency of online control through development are supported, at least partly, by a greater capacity to generate internal representations of action can be found from evidence that the development of motor imagery (MI), an experimental protocol thought to elucidate the integrity of the action representation that ordinarily precedes movement (Decety, Jeannerod, & Prablanc, 1989; Parsons, 1994; Sirigu et al., 1996), follows a similar maturational timeline to that of online control.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text