Elsevier

Biological Psychiatry

Volume 72, Issue 12, 15 December 2012, Pages 1012-1019
Biological Psychiatry

Archival Report
Attention-Related Pearce-Kaye-Hall Signals in Basolateral Amygdala Require the Midbrain Dopaminergic System

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.023Get rights and content

Background

Neural activity in basolateral amygdala has recently been shown to reflect surprise or attention as predicted by the Pearce-Kaye-Hall model (PKH)—an influential model of associative learning. Theoretically, a PKH attentional signal originates in prediction errors of the kind associated with phasic firing of dopamine neurons. This requirement for prediction errors, coupled with projections from the midbrain dopamine system into basolateral amygdala, suggests that the PKH signal in amygdala may depend on dopaminergic input.

Methods

To test this, we recorded single unit activity in basolateral amygdala in rats with 6-hydroxydopamine or sham lesions of the ipsilateral midbrain region. Neurons were recorded as the rats performed a task previously used to demonstrate both dopaminergic reward prediction errors and attentional signals in basolateral amygdala neurons.

Results

We found that neurons recorded in sham lesioned rats exhibited the same attention-related PKH signal observed in previous studies. By contrast, neurons recorded in rats with ipsilateral 6-hydroxydopamine lesions failed to show attentional signaling.

Conclusions

These results indicate a linkage between the neural instantiations of the basolateral complex of the amygdala attentional signal and dopaminergic prediction errors. Such a linkage would have important implications for understanding both normal and aberrant learning and behavior, particularly in diseases thought to have a primary effect on dopamine systems, such as addiction and schizophrenia.

Section snippets

Subjects, Behavioral Apparatus, and Testing

Male Long-Evans rats served as subjects, tested at the University of Maryland, Baltimore, in accordance with School of Medicine and National Institutes of Health guidelines. Equipment and training were identical to prior experiments (3). Briefly, training used aluminum chambers. A central odor port was located above two adjacent fluid wells on a panel in the right wall of each chamber. On each trial, nosepoke into the odor port resulted in delivery of the odor cue. One of three different odors

Results

Neuronal activity in ABL was recorded while rats performed a choice task. On each trial, rats were required to nosepoke at an odor port where one of three possible odors could be presented. Odors predicted reward at one of two adjacent fluid wells situated below the odor port (Figure 1A). Odor 1 signaled that the sucrose reward would be delivered in the left well (forced-left), odor 2 signaled that it would be delivered in the right well (forced-right), and odor 3 signaled that it would be made

Discussion

The current study replicates the attentional signal in ABL previously shown in rats (3) and humans (4) and further demonstrates its dependence on the dopaminergic system. What role does dopamine play in the construction of the attentional signal in ABL? An obvious possibility is that the latter has its origin in the signed prediction error signals widely reported in midbrain DA neurons (8, 9, 10, 11, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39). In addition to inverting the polarity of neural activity for

References (68)

  • F. Ambroggi et al.

    Basolateral amygdala neurons facilitate reward-seeking behavior by exciting nucleus accumbens neurons

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • A.C. Courville et al.

    Bayesian theories of conditioning in a changing world

    Trends Cogn Sci

    (2006)
  • P.R. Corlett et al.

    Toward a neurobiology of delusions

    Prog Neurobiol

    (2010)
  • M. Torregrossa et al.

    Aberrant learning and memory in addiction

    Neurobiol Learn Mem

    (2011)
  • M.R. Roesch et al.

    Neural correlates of variations in event processing during learning in basolateral amygdala

    J Neurosci

    (2010)
  • J. Li et al.

    Differential roles of human striatum and amygdala in associative learning

    Nat Neurosci

    (2011)
  • C. Herry et al.

    Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala

    J Neurosci

    (2007)
  • K.M. Tye et al.

    Amygdala neural encoding in the absence of reward during extinction

    J Neurosci

    (2010)
  • J.P. Johansen et al.

    Neural substrates for expectation-modulated fear learning in the amygdala and periaqueductal gray

    Nat Neurosci

    (2010)
  • P.R. Montague et al.

    A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive hebbian learning

    J Neurosci

    (1996)
  • J. Mirenowicz et al.

    Importance of unpredictability for reward responses in primate dopamine neurons

    J Neurophysiol

    (1994)
  • W.-X. Pan et al.

    Dopamine cells respond to predicted events during classical conditioning: Evidence for eligibility traces in the reward-learning network

    J Neurosci

    (2005)
  • K. D'Ardenne et al.

    BOLD responses reflecting dopaminergic signals in the human ventral tegmental area

    Science

    (2008)
  • R.A. Rescorla et al.

    A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement

  • W. Schultz et al.

    A neural substrate for prediction and reward

    Science

    (1997)
  • R.S. Sutton

    Learning to predict by the method of temporal difference

    Machine Learning

    (1988)
  • J.M. Pearce et al.

    A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli

    Psychol Rev

    (1980)
  • N.J. Mackintosh

    A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement

    Psychol Rev

    (1975)
  • J.M. Pearce et al.

    Predictive accuracy and stimulus associability: Development of a model for Pavlovian learning

  • K.M. Tye et al.

    Rapid strengthening of thalamo-amygdala synapses mediates cue-reward learning

    Nature

    (2008)
  • G. Schoenbaum et al.

    Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning

    J Neurosci

    (1999)
  • J.J. Patton et al.

    The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning

    Nature

    (2006)
  • A. Fontanini et al.

    Distinct subtypes of basolateral amygdala neurons reflect palatability and reward

    J Neurosci

    (2009)
  • Y. Sugase-Miyamoto et al.

    Neuronal signals in the monkey basolateral amygdala during reward schedules

    J Neurosci

    (2005)
  • Cited by (33)

    • The basolateral amygdala and lateral hypothalamus bias learning towards motivationally significant events

      2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similarly, in another study, BLA neurons increased firing rate with an unexpected change in reward magnitude or delay, exhibiting a short burst of increased activity whether these changes meant more or less reward, or delivered over a shorter or longer timescale [24]. This is reminiscent of an unsigned attentional signal [24], strengthening the idea that BLA is concerned with the specific outcomes predicted by environmental stimuli and their motivational significance [24,25]. Altogether, these studies provide evidence to support the BLA as an associative structure important for learning about motivationally significant events by encoding sensory-specific information about these events, including their current predictive status.

    • Prediction errors and valence: From single units to multidimensional encoding in the amygdala

      2021, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Importantly, these signals seem to contribute to the attentional changes proposed by the amended Pearce-Hall classical conditioning model, in which changes in firing are influenced by a history of prediction errors over several trials [78,80,81]. Interestingly, however, subsequent results illustrated that these unsigned signals in BLA were partially dependent on the midbrain dopamine system, which classically signals signed reward prediction errors, suggesting that the two signals are intertwined [78]. Both of these signals are in stark contrast to firing observed in CeA in rats performing the same size-delay task, where single neuron firing in CeA contributed to signaling negative—but not positive—prediction errors.

    • What is dopamine doing in model-based reinforcement learning?

      2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
    • Dynamic resource allocation during reinforcement learning accounts for ramping and phasic dopamine activity

      2020, Neural Networks
      Citation Excerpt :

      In Fig. 5B, experimental (or essential) stimuli, non-essential stimuli, and the reward are indicated by a red-, blue-, or orange-colored number, respectively. Based on previous reversal studies showing that large prediction errors occurring at the beginning of each block enhance the learning rate (Esber, et al., 2012; Li et al., 2011), we made the prediction error generated by the previous outcome adjust the learning rate (see Section 2.2). In this reversal experiment, the environment remains constant until the animal exits from the bridge, while the environment after the animal making its choice changes across blocks (Fig. 5A).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Authors GRE and MRR contributed equally to this work.

    View full text